PEERS Preschool
Educational Environment Rating System (staat volledig op Internet)
Susan B. Neuman, New
York University
Keuze voor uitgebalanceerd
kleuteronderwijs : combinatie van leerling-geïnitieerde activiteiten en
leerkracht-gedirigeerde. Leerkracht-gestuurde: b.v. dosis expliciete
taalstimulering naast impliciete. Wat dit laatste betreft lezen we o.a.: In
particular, sociocultural theory highlights the importance of adult guidance as
children work to master skills they cannot yet understand on their own, but can
learn with support and guidance (Vygotsky, 1978).
The Preschool Educational Environment Rating System (PEERS)
is a measure designed to examine the quality of instruction in preschool
settings. Unlike other rating scales, it
not only measures the environment, it also examines both how teachers construct
their classroom for instruction and the quality of the enactment of
instruction. Designed on behalf of the
Albert Shanker Institute for a collaboration with the Saint Louis Public School
System (SLPSS), the PEERS is an evidence-based measure that can be used as an
observational tool by administrators to more fully understand and assess the
environments and instruction they provide to their students, with the ultimate
goal of improving childrens academic outcomes.
DESCRIPTION OF THE MEASURE
The PEERS is a high quality, evidence-based measure of
preschool quality. It is informed by several theoretical perspectives,
primarily that of ecological psychology (Day, 1983; Gump, 1989). This
perspective suggests that the organization and complexity of the environment
plays a central role in a childs learning and development. The PEERS is also grounded
in the assumptions of attachment theory and sociocultural theory. The former
emphasizes the importance of inter-personal relationships to the development of
childrens social, emotional, and
cognitive abilities (Bowlby, 2008). The latter stresses the importance of
interactions between developing children and their cultural surroundings. In particular,
sociocultural theory highlights the importance of adult guidance as children
work to master skills they cannot yet understand on their own, but can learn
with support and guidance (Vygotsky, 1978).
In line with these theoretical perspectives, the PEERS is
designed to gather information about two essential aspects of the preschool
classroom: the environment in which children are learning, and the instruction
they receive. Within these broad categories, nine sub-categories are housed.
The items included in the PEERS are based firmly in recent rigorous research,
and measure factors that have been strongly linked to the development of core
skills (especially literacy) and/or later academic performance, as detailed
below.
PEERS Categories
The PEERS begins with an observation record, which allows
for the collection of basic information required for data review and analysis.
This includes classroom information (teacher, school, district, number of
adults and children in the room, etc.) and information about the observation
(observer name, date, time, etc.) It also includes space for the observer to add
comments or notes. Following the observation record, the PEERS contains items
organized into two large sections: the environment and instruction.
Environment
In line with the assumptions of ecological psychology, a
large body of research indicates the powerful impact of the environment on a
childs learning and development. In particular, it has been found that both
patterns of activity and engagement are influenced by access to materials, as
well as the organization and complexity of the setting.
Within the environment section of the measure, items are
grouped into the following five sub-sections: classroom organization and
environment; planning and documentation; lesson plans; materials and displays;
and books and computers. Each of these sub-sections contains measures that
highlight recent research in that area. For example, a body of studies suggests
the importance of classroom organization, the first aspect of classroom quality
measured in the environment portion of the PEERS. Research has long indicated
that the arrangement and organization of physical spaces influence human
behavior (Greenman, 1988; Mcgrew, 1970; Phyfe-Perkins, 1980). Studies on the
design of early childhood classrooms also indicate the importance of a safe and
child-centered environment to the development of competency (Trancik & Evans,
1995), as well as the importance of offering both large and small-group instruction
(e.g., Foorman & Torgesen, 2001; Montie, Claxton, & Lockhart, 2007;
Morrow & Smith, 1990).
The next sub-section of the PEERS asks assessors to observe
aspects of classroom planning and documentation. Items in this sub-section
highlight the importance of offering a wide variety of learning activities in preschools.
For instance, child-directed and child-initiated activities arehave been found
to be crucial to learning (Marcon, 1999), particularly when it comes to
language development (Montie et al., 2007). Additionally, the benefits of deep,
ongoing, teacher-led investigations are also captured in this section of the PEERS.
Finally, when considering classroom planning and documentation, it is important
to assess the extent to which portfolios and similar assessments are used to
monitor childrens progress; research suggests that such methods are beneficial
to tracking and fostering childrens learning (Gronlund & Engel, 2001; Lynch
& A., 2001; Mills, 1994).
The third sub-section of the PEERS gathers information about
lesson plans, examining in more detail the types of instructional activities
presented to preschoolers. Items align with research about activities that
promote young childrens learning, including shared book reading (Crain- Thoreson
& Dale, 1999; Mol, Bus, & de Jong, 2009) and opportunities to engage
with emergent writing, which has been shown to predict later reading and writing
ability (Moody, Justice, & Cabell, 2010; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 2003).
Additional items in this sub-section are designed to capture the extent to
which other subject areas that are important for future success are integrated
into the daily plan and aligned with both the curriculum and broader learning
goals.
These include math (Cross, Woods, & Schweingruber, 2009;
National Mathematics Advisory Panel, 2008; Sarama & Clements, 2009; Sarama,
Lange, Clements, & Wolfe, 2012), science (Chaille & Britain, 1997;
Gallas, 1995; Zimmerman, 2000), music (Campbell & Scott-Kassner, 2013;
Črnčec, Wilson, & Prior, 2006; Strait, Parbery-Clark, OConnell, & Kraus,
2013), art (E. P. Cohen & Gainer, 1995; Thompson, 1995), and movement
(Campbell & Scott-Kassner, 2013; Lorenzo-Lasa, Ideishi, & Ideishi,
2007).
The sub-section of the PEERS designed to capture the
materials and displays also includes items that highlight recent research. For
example, research indicates that children in preschools with a greater number
and variety of accessible materials have been found to have higher cognitive scores
(Montie et al., 2007). This may be in part explained by studies indicating that
children in classrooms that include a wide variety of accessible tools, books,
and materials both read more (Neuman & Roskos, 1992) and increase their
literacy abilities (Neuman & Roskos, 1990). It is generally understood that
children are more likely to constructively use materials that are readily
accessible to them and organized into conceptually related groups, promoting development
across learning domains. A body of research indicates that well-organized
settings foster development and learning, supporting this understanding (D. K.
Cohen, Raudenbush, & Ball, 2003; Tharp & Gallimore, 1989; Wachs, 1987).
Another key item in this sub-section measures the extent to which environmental
print exists throughout the classroom (e.g., objects labeled at eye level,
print included for both functional and play purposes). A body of research supports
this item. Indeed, environmental print has repeatedly been found to promote
literacy activities (Morrow & Smith, 1990; Neuman, Celano, Greco, & Shue,
2001; Prior & Gerard, 2004; Wolfersberger, Reutzel, Sudweeks, & Fawson,
2004), particularly when adults actively engage children with print in the
classroom (Neuman & Roskos, 1993).
Instruction
Along with elements of the classroom environment, the instruction
provided to children has repeatedly been demonstrated to affect their
development and learning across content areas. Within the second half of the
PEERS, items are grouped into the following four sub-sections: supervision and
management; climate; responsive instruction; and facilitating home support for learning.
In line with the assumptions of attachment theory and sociocultural theory, the
evidence for most items in this portion of the measure is derived from the
interactions and relationships between teachers and their students.
The first sub-section in the instruction portion of the
PEERS evaluates the supervision and management of the classroom. Several key
items that are known to influence learning are measured through observation of
the behaviors and interactions of children and teachers in the classroom, as
well as classroom rules and routines. For example, childrens internalization
of rules and routines, and their peaceful movement through the day are
evaluated. Although little research has been done on the topic in preschool
classrooms, studies focused on classroom management in the upper elementary
grades indicate the importance of a peaceful and well-run classroom (see Carter
& Doyle, 2006). Along the same lines, the quality of teacher intervention has
been shown to affect childrens ability to independently, peacefully, and
effectively resolve conflicts with their peers (see Slaby, 1995). When
preschoolers can employ social problem- solving skills, more time is left for
play and learning.
Next, the PEERS builds on research showing the importance of
the classroom climate . For example, the observer is directed to gauge the
active listening and empathy of the teacher. Teacher-child interactions rich in
these elements foster childrens socio-emotional competence. This, in turn,
provides a foundation that supports development. More specifically,
high-quality interactions with teachers and other caregivers foster childrens
capacity for intimacy and empathy, self-esteem, impulse control and
self-regulation, creativity, language acquisition, and ability to problem-solve
(Ostrosky, Gaffney, & Thomas, 2006, p. 183). Research indicates that these
developmental gains then translate into academic success. The work of Robert
Pianta (e.g., Hamre & Pianta, 2001; R. Pianta, Belsky, Houts, Morrison,
& the National Institute of Child Health and Human Developments Early
Child Care Research Network, 2007; R. Pianta et al., 2005; R. C. Pianta &
Stuhlman, 2004) has been particularly influential, spearheading a body of work
in this area. The PEERS also measures the engagement, happiness, trust and
respect that children demonstrate as a result of these interactions.
Warm and responsive instruction, also measured by the PEERS,
has been found to promote learning as well. For example, Connor and colleagues
found that first grade children with more responsive teachers demonstrated
stronger vocabulary and decoding skills at the end of the year (Connor, Son,
Hindman, & Morrison, 2005; see also Graue, Clements, Reynolds, & Niles,
2004). Intentional efforts to expand childrens vocabulary also play an important
role in facilitating learning. A body of work indicates that such efforts can
increase childrens vocabulary knowledge
(Hargrave & Sénéchal, 2000; Marulis & Neuman, 2010; Neuman, Newman,
& Dwyer, 2011; Robbins & Ehri, 1994). This increased vocabulary ability
then aids in early reading ability, which, in turn, bootstraps achievement across
school subjects. Key research by Keith Stanovich suggesting a reciprocal
relationship between reading and cognitive efficiency may well explain this
phenomenon (Stanovich, 1986; West, Stanovich, & Mitchell, 1993).
Bijlage: Neuman wijst
ook op belang van voldoende expliciete taalinstructie
Ook dosis expliciete
taalstimulering (woordenschat e.d.) is belangrijk Marulis & Neuman :
Vocabulary Intervention : uit review-studie blijkt dat ook voldoende expliciete
stimulering heel belangrijk is
Review of Educational Research September 2010,
Vol. 80, No. 3, pp. 300335
The Effects of Vocabulary Intervention on
Young Childrens Word Learning: A Meta-Analysis Loren M. Marulis and Susan B.
Neuman University of Michigan
This meta-analysis examines the effects of
vocabulary interventions on pre-K and kindergarten childrens oral language
development. The authors quantitatively reviewed 67 studies and 216 effect
sizes to better understand the impact of training on word learning. Results
indicated an overall effect size of .88, demonstrating, on average, a gain of
nearly one standard deviation on vocabulary measures. Moderator analyses
reported greater effects for trained adults in providing the treatment,
combined pedagogical strategies that included explicit and implicit
instruction, and author-created measures compared to standardized measures.
Turning to the
instructional features of these vocabulary interventions, our analysis revealed
that pedagogical approach appeared to make a difference. Programs that used
explicit instruction deliberately either through explanation of words or key
examples were associated with larger effect sizes than those that taught words
implicitly. In addition, programs that combined explicit and implicit
instruction, enabling students to be introduced to words followed by meaningful
practice and review, demonstrated even larger effects. These studies stand in
contrast to those that used implicit instruction alone, which was found to be
less effective. They confirm the recommendations of the National Reading Panel
(2000), which called for providing direct instruction in vocabulary with
multiple exposures in rich contexts. Given that this meta-analysis focused on
many different training programs, these results should generalize across
specific types of programs. Furthermore, there is evidence to suggest the
benefits of explicit instruction may not be limited to word learning (Klahr
& Nigam, 2004; Rittle-Johnson, 2006; Star & Rittle-Johnson, 2008).
|